Skip to main content

Discriminant content validity of a theoretical domains framework questionnaire for use in implementation research

Abstract

Background

To improve the implementation of innovations in healthcare settings, it is important to understand factors influencing healthcare professionals’ behaviors. We aimed to develop a generic questionnaire in English and in Dutch assessing the 14 domains of behavioral determinants from the revised TDF (Cane et al., 2012) that can be tailored to suit different targets, actions, contexts, and times of interest, and to investigate questionnaire items’ discriminant content validity.

Methods

We identified existing questionnaires including items assessing constructs within TDF domains and developed new items where needed. Nineteen judges allocated 79 items to one or more TDF domains. One-sample t-tests were used to examine the discriminant content validity of each item, i.e., whether items measured intended domains or whether items measured a combination of domains.

Results

We identified items judged to discriminately measure 11 out of 14 domains. Items measuring the domains Reinforcement, Goals, and Behavioral regulation were judged to measure a combination of domains.

Conclusions

We have developed a questionnaire in English and in Dutch able to discriminately assess the majority of TDF domains. The results partly support Cane et al.’s (2012) 14-domain validation of the TDF and suggest that Michie et al.’s (2005) 12-domain original version might be more applicable in developing a TDF-based questionnaire. The identified items provide a robust basis for developing a questionnaire to measure TDF-based determinants of healthcare professionals’ implementation behaviors to suit different targets, actions, contexts, and times. Future research should investigate the concurrent and predictive validity and reliability of such a questionnaire in practice.

Peer Review reports

Background

Healthcare professionals routinely deliver pharmacological and behavior change interventions to their patients to promote health and prevent disease. However, as the evidence-base for effective interventions is continuously developing, the transfer of such evidence into routine practice often does not happen as desired[1–3]. For example, primary care-based interventions for increasing physical activity (PA) are effective[4–7], yet rates of PA counseling by healthcare professionals are suboptimal[8, 9], as is the fidelity of delivery of PA interventions[2, 10, 11]. This gap between research and practice reduces the impact that effective behavior change interventions can have on public health[12, 13]. Implementation research aims to bridge this gap by investigating methods to promote healthcare professionals’ uptake of research findings, including the study of factors influencing healthcare professional behavior[14, 15].

Improving the adoption and implementation of evidence-based interventions into routine practice involves changes in healthcare professionals’ behaviors that may be influenced by a range of individual, organizational, and social factors[16–20]. Identifying the key factors associated with healthcare professional behavior can provide a basis for developing interventions to help healthcare professionals to use research findings more effectively[14]. Given the range of potential factors associated with behavior, many advocate the use of theory to guide the selection of factors to investigate[15, 21–23]. In addition, the UK Medical Research Council guidance on developing and evaluating complex interventions recommends the use of theory in the intervention development phase[24]. The advantages of a theory-based approach are numerous: theory allows for a shared understanding, for the development of a cumulative science that limits the re-invention of existing concepts, and importantly is based on constructs which have been investigated, for which measures can be validated and standardized and have been shown to provide a useful account of behavior[25]. Furthermore, investigating the relationship between theory-based factors and healthcare professional behavior provides an opportunity to identify factors that can be targeted by implementation interventions to change healthcare professional behavior[15, 23, 26, 27].

The number and heterogeneity of potential theories that might be used to guide implementation research poses a challenge to researchers wanting to assess and identify theory-based factors underlying healthcare professional behavior[22, 28–30]. The Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF)[31] was developed as an integrative framework of theories of behavior change to overcome these challenges. The framework includes 12 theoretical domains of potential behavioral determinants and provides exemplar questions for the theoretical assessment of implementation problems. The framework has been used in a number of studies and was demonstrated to be useful for the development of qualitative[32, 33] and quantitative[34–36] measurement tools to assess potential implementation behavior determinants. However, factor analysis implied that only one out of these three questionnaires was able to measure the theoretical domains independently[36]. Furthermore, the questionnaires were developed to assess determinants of specific implementation behaviors in specific settings (i.e., tobacco use prevention and smoking cessation in dental healthcare[34], smoking cessation in maternal care[35], and different types of patient safety behaviors in hospitals[36]) and internal consistency reliability was low[34] or could be improved[35, 36].

Since its original development, the consensus study that produced the TDF[31] has been validated, leading to Cane et al.’s[37] refined TDF. It extends the original TDF to include the following 14 domains: Knowledge; Skills; Social/professional role and identity; Beliefs about capabilities; Optimism; Beliefs about consequences; Reinforcement; Intentions; Goals; Memory, attention and decision processes; Environmental context and resources; Social influences; Emotions; and Behavioral regulation. Main differences between the original and the revised framework include the separation of the domain Optimism from the domain Beliefs about capabilities and the domain Reinforcement from the domain Beliefs about consequences. In addition, the domain Motivation and goals was divided into two separate domains, i.e., Intentions and Goals, and the domain Nature of the behaviors was omitted in the revised framework. Although the framework is suggested to be useful for the development of theory-based questionnaires for use in implementation research, the content of the TDF has not yet been validated on item level. Therefore, it is not clear whether questionnaire items based on this recent version of the framework will be able to measure the 14 domains independently.

In the present study we aimed to develop a questionnaire assessing the 14 TDF domains, worded in such a way to provide researchers the capacity to tailor the items to the targets, actions, contexts and times of interest[38], whilst retaining the essential theoretical content in each item. Furthermore, we aimed to test the discriminant content validity of each item within the questionnaire.

Methods

Participants

Fifty-eight academics from the Netherlands were approached with details of the study and nineteen agreed to participate (response rate of 33%). They were either involved as experts in the field of behavior change, development of health behavior change interventions, or implementation of interventions in healthcare settings. They were recruited via the authors’ networks. The sample size was based on estimates of between three and 20 participants as adequate for judgment tasks[39, 40]. We included academics (instead of healthcare professionals) in this study, because the discriminant content validation (DCV) exercise of allocating items to TDF domains requires theoretical knowledge and experience with the specific domains.

Materials

We developed a questionnaire that initially included 79 items assessing each of the domains through their related key constructs (see Additional file1). Constructs within domains were selected based on conceptual relatedness to the content of the domain (i.e., Knowledge, Procedural knowledge, Skills, Professional role, and Memory); inclusion in relevant theories frequently used in the field of behavior change (and thus ready access to existing items): the Theory of Planned Behavior[41] (i.e., Perceived behavioral control, Attitudes, Subjective norm, and Intention) and Social Cognitive Theory[42] (i.e., Self-efficacy, Outcome expectancies, and Social support); existence of validated scales (i.e., Optimism, Pessimism, Action planning, Attention, Affect, Stress, Automaticity, and Self-monitoring); and/or relevance to the implementation of PA interventions in routine healthcare by mapping factors resulting from previous research[43, 44] onto the TDF domains. JP and JMH independently identified that the constructs Reinforcement, Priority, Resources/materials, and Descriptive norm were salient in the previous PA-based research and thus these constructs were also included as construct-indicators of their respective domains.

Items measuring constructs within the domains Knowledge, Beliefs about capabilities, Optimism, Beliefs about consequences, Intentions, Social influences, Emotion, and Behavioral regulation were adapted from previously published questionnaires (i.e.,[34, 35, 41, 42, 45–53]). Given lack of available questionnaires in the literature for some domains, new items were created for the domains Skills, Social/professional role and identity, Reinforcement, and Environmental context and resources. With regard to the domain Goals, items were newly developed for the construct Priority (as none could be located in the literature), while items measuring the construct Action planning were adapted from a previously published questionnaire[46]. With regard to the domain Memory, attention, and decision making, items measuring the construct Attention were adapted from a previously published questionnaire[51] and items measuring the construct Memory were newly developed. New items were developed based on discussions between JP and JMH. These discussions were informed by the academic literature on the concept and definition of specific domains and constructs, questions to identify behavior change processes as formulated by Michie et al.[31], and themes emerging from interviews on the implementation of PA interventions[43]. WAG and MRC supervised the development of the questionnaire and reviewed items’ face validity.

To develop a questionnaire which could be used by researchers in different fields of implementation research, items were formulated in a generic way using a '[action] in [context, time] with [target]’ construction based on the 'TACT principle’[38], whereby researchers can specify the target, action, context, and time relevant to their research. The questionnaire was developed in English, then translated to Dutch and back-translated to English by an independent translator. The small amount of differences between the original and back-translated version of the questionnaire were discussed and adaptations were made.

Procedure

In May and June 2012 participants were sent an email including the link to the online DCV exercise[54, 55]. After one and two weeks non respondents received a reminder. Participants were provided with the aim of the study and an explanation of the DCV exercise. Then, they were asked to report their expertise on each of the 14 TDF domains on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = I am a layman with regard to this domain; 7 = I am an expert with regard to this domain).

We used Cane et al.’s[37] definitions of the 14 TDF domains (see Table 1), which were presented at the top of each rating page. The items of the questionnaire were listed below the definitions, in a random order. Participants were asked to consider carefully the meaning of each item and allocate it to the domain they perceived the item measures using the domain definitions provided. To determine whether items were deemed to discriminately measure domains or if they measure a combination of domains, participants were asked to allocate each of the 79 items to up to three domains. Upon allocating items, judges were asked to rate their confidence in each allocation between 0% and 100% (0% = not at all confident; 100% = extremely confident). For example, a judge could allocate an item to the domain Knowledge and rate their confidence 60% and allocate the same item to the domain Skills and rate their confidence 20%.

Table 1 Definitions of the domains of the TDF[37]1

Data analysis

Classification of items

Ratings for matching items and domains (i.e., items judged to assess the domain they were designed to assess) were coded 1 (a 'match’), whereas items judged to assess a different domain were coded -1 (a 'no match’); missing variables were scored 0. Each judgment was multiplied by its accompanied confidence rating (e.g., .20, .40, .80). As a consequence, the weighted judgments ranged from -1 to 1.

DCV analysis

Following Dixon et al.[54, 55], we used one-sample one-tailed t-tests to investigate whether each item was classified by the judges to represent the domain that the item aimed to measure. Judges were provided with three possibilities to allocate an item to a domain, therefore, the sum of the three weighted judgments was used for the one-sample t-tests. An item was classified as measuring a domain if its weighted judgment against that domain was significantly greater than zero (p < .05)[54]. The false discovery rate controlling procedure[57] was used to correct for multiple tests. Items that were classified to the correct (i.e., intended) domain were included in the final questionnaire, whereas items that were allocated to more than one domain or that were classified to a domain other than the intended domain were not included. Analyses were performed in IBM SPSS Statistics version 19.0[58].

Inter-rater agreement

A generalization of Cohen’s kappa (i.e., Light’s Kappa[59]) was calculated to assess agreement between judges across their allocation of all items to domains. For this calculation, we used the first domain that judges selected to represent the item. This was justified as the data indicated that judges used the first selected domain as the most preferable domain (i.e., domain with the highest confidence ratings) to allocate an item to. As a consequence, the 79 items were scored between 1 and 14 (representing the domain it was allocated to) for each judge separately. This resulted in a data matrix composed of 79 rows (i.e., the items) and 19 columns (i.e., the judges). We also assessed inter-rater agreement for allocation of items to each domain. For this calculation, the 79 items were scored between 1 and 0 for each domain separately (representing if it was selected to the specific domain or not) and for each judge separately. This resulted in 14 data matrices, one for each domain, consisting of 79 rows and 19 columns. These analyses were repeated for the final set of items that was selected based on the DCV analysis. In line with previous research, κ-values of between .00 and .20 were labeled as slight agreement, values from .21 to .40 as fair agreement, values from .41 to .60 as moderate agreement, values from .61 to .80 as substantial, and values from .81 to 1.00 as almost perfect[60]. Analyses were performed in the R software environment[61], using the R-package 'Psy’[62].

Ethics

The Medical Ethics Committee of the Leiden University Medical Centre gave ethics approval for this study (reference number NV/CME 09/081).

Results

Judges’ expertise in the use of domains

Descriptive statistics of judges’ expertise in the use of each domain are shown in Table 2. Mean scores indicated that judges had at least some expertise on each domain. On average, judges rated that they had most expertise on the domains Intentions and Goals, whereas lowest expertise ratings were given to the domains Social/professional role and identity, and Memory, attention, and decision processes. Only three judges indicated to be a layman on, respectively, one, two, and seven domains.

Table 2 Judges’ expertise on domains

Neither judges’ expertise with TDF domains nor their academic level (i.e., PhD student, PhD, Professor) was related to their performance on the classification of items to domains calculated as the number of 'matches’. Pearson’s correlations were respectively r = -.35 (p = .14) and r = -.16 (p = .52).

DCV results

Table 3 shows the results of the DCV analysis. Of 79 items, 32 were classified as measuring the intended domain and therefore included in the final questionnaire. Forty-seven items were allocated to more than one domain, of which 39 items were allocated to the intended domain as well as additional domains, while eight items were classified as measuring a domain other than the item aimed to measure. Table 4 shows Kappa values for the agreement between judges based on all 79 items of the initial questionnaire and the 32 items included in the final questionnaire. The final lists of items measuring TDF domains are shown in Table 5 (English) and Table 6 (Dutch).

Table 3 DCV analysis of the questionnaire
Table 4 Light’s κ-values for all items and the items included in the final questionnaire
Table 5 Final list of items measuring TDF domains (English)
Table 6 Final list of items measuring TDF domains (Dutch)

Knowledge

The domain Knowledge was defined as 'an awareness of the existence of something’[37]. Of the six Knowledge items included in the DCV exercise, four items were classified as measuring the domain Knowledge (Table 4) and were included in the final questionnaire. Two items were allocated to more than one domain. In addition to the domain Knowledge, these items were amongst others allocated to the domain Skills. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was substantial when including all items (κ = .76; 95% C.I. .63-.87; Table 4) and almost perfect for the 32 final items (κ = .88; 95% C.I. .77-.96; Table 4).

Skills

The domain Skills was defined as 'an ability or proficiency acquired through practice’[37]. Three out of four Skills items included in the DCV were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. In addition to the domain Skills, nine judges allocated the item 'I have the proficiency to…’ to the domain Beliefs about capabilities. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .58; 95% C.I. .35-.71; Table 4), while substantial agreement was found for the 32 final items (κ = .80; 95% C.I. .73-.87; Table 4).

Social/professional role and identity

The domain Social/professional role and identity was defined as 'a coherent set of behaviors and displayed personal qualities of an individual in a social or work setting’[37]. All four Social/professional role and identity items included in the DCV were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was moderate with all items included (κ = .59; 95% C.I. .37-.75; Table 4) and almost perfect for the 32 final items (κ = .86; 95% C.I. .72-.93; Table 4).

Beliefs about capabilities

The domain Beliefs about capabilities was defined as 'acceptance of the truth, reality, or validity about an ability, talent, or facility that a person can put to constructive use’[37]. Six Beliefs about capabilities items were included in the DCV exercise. The three items containing the word 'confident’ were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. The items measuring the difficulty and possibility of [action] in [context, time] with [target] were allocated to more than one domain. In addition to the domain Beliefs about capabilities, they were often allocated to the domain Skills. The item 'How much control do you have over…’ was allocated to the intended domain, but also to the domains Skills and Behavioral regulation. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .55; 95% C.I. .41-.71; Table 4), while substantial agreement was found for the 32 final items (κ = .73; 95% C.I. .60-.81; Table 4).

Optimism

The domain Optimism was defined as 'the confidence that things will happen for the best or that desired goals will be attained’[37]. Two out of six Optimism items included in the DCV were classified as measuring the domain Optimism (Table 3). These were included in the final questionnaire. Four items were allocated to more than one domain, including the domains Beliefs about capabilities and Beliefs about consequences. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was moderate with all items included (κ = .60; 95% C.I. .49-.69; Table 4) and substantial for the final 32 items (κ = .68; 95% C.I. .63-.72; Table 4).

Beliefs about consequences

The domain Beliefs about consequences was defined as 'acceptance of the truth, reality, or validity about outcomes of a behavior in a given situation’[37]. Of the four Beliefs about consequences items included in the DCV, only two items were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and included in the questionnaire. These were the items measuring the construct Outcome expectancies. The two items measuring the construct Attitudes were allocated to a variety of domains, including Social/professional role and identity and Optimism. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .49; 95% C.I. .34-.62; Table 4), while substantial agreement was found for the final 32 items (κ = .70; 95% C.I. .67-.73; Table 4).

Reinforcement

The domain Reinforcement was defined as 'increasing the probability of a response by arranging a dependent relationship, or contingency, between the response and a given stimulus’[37]. The DCV exercise included three items intended to measure Reinforcement, but none of them was classified as measuring the domain (Table 3) and so none of them was included in the final questionnaire. The item '…I get financial reimbursement’ was, in addition to the intended domain, allocated to the domain Beliefs about consequences. Two items were classified as measuring domains they were not intended to measure. The item '…I get recognition from professionals who are important to me’ was classified as measuring the domain Social influences and the item '…I feel like I am making a difference’ was classified as measuring the domain Beliefs about consequences. Five judges did not allocate any item to the domain. Without these judges taken into account Cohen’s kappa indicated moderate agreement (κ = .59; 95% C.I. .50-.68; Table 4).

Intentions

The domain Intentions was defined as 'a conscious decision to perform a behavior or a resolve to act in a certain way’[37]. All four items included in the DCV to measure Intentions were classified as measuring the domain (Table 3) and included in the final questionnaire. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was substantial with all items included (κ = .75; 95% C.I. .56-.87; Table 4) and almost perfect for the final 32 items (κ = .93; 95% C.I. .89-1.00; Table 4).

Goals

The domain Goals was defined as 'mental representations of outcomes or end states that an individual wants to achieve’[37]. Eight Goals items were included in the DCV exercise. None of them were classified to the right domain (Table 3) and thus Goals items were not included in the final questionnaire. Items measuring the construct Priority were classified as measuring the domain Memory, attention, and decision processes. The four items measuring the construct Action planning were included in the DCV as measuring both the domain Goals and Behavioral regulation. They were not classified as measuring these two domains, because they were also often allocated to the domain Intentions. Three judges did not allocate items to the domain. Without these judges taken into account kappa indicated slight agreement (κ = .11; 95% C.I. .07-.14; Table 4).

Memory, attention, and decision processes

The domain Memory, attention, and decision processes was defined as 'the ability to retain information, focus selectively on aspects of the environment and choose between two or more alternatives’[37]. Eight items were included in the DCV exercise to measure the domain Memory, attention, and decision processes. Four of these items were classified to measure the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. Two items were allocated to more than one domain and two items measuring the construct Memory were classified as measuring a domain other than they were intended to measure (i.e., Knowledge and Beliefs about capabilities). The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was substantial with all items included (κ = .63; 95% C.I. .48-.75; Table 4) and almost perfect for the final 32 items (κ = .85; 95% C.I. .79-.90; Table 4).

Environmental context and resources

The domain Environmental context and resources was defined as 'any circumstance of a person’s situation or environment that discourages or encourages the development of skills and abilities, independence, social competence, and adaptive behavior’[37]. Eight items were included in the DCV to measure this domain, while only two items were classified as measuring the domain (Table 3) and therefore could be included in the final questionnaire. Other items, not including the word 'socio-political context’ were, in addition to the intended domain, foremost allocated to the domains Skills, Social/professional role and identity, and Social influences. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .48; 95% C.I. .34-.65; Table 4), while almost perfect agreement was found for the final 32 items (κ = .82; 95% C.I. .73-.87; Table 4).

Social influences

The domain Social influences was defined as 'those interpersonal processes that can cause individuals to change their thoughts, feelings, or behaviors’[37]. Two out of eight Social influences items included in the DCV, were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and therefore included in the final questionnaire. These were the items measuring the construct Subjective norm. In addition to the domain Social influences, the other six items were mostly allocated to the domains Social/professional role and identity and Environmental context and resources. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was moderate with all items included (κ = .53; 95% C.I. .43-.67; Table 4) and substantial for the final 32 items (κ = .78; 95% C.I. .69-.86; Table 4).

Emotion

The domain Emotion was defined as 'a complex reaction pattern, involving experiential, behavioral, and physiological elements, by which the individual attempts to deal with a personally significant matter or event’[37]. Of the four Emotion items included in the DCV exercise, the two items measuring the construct Stress were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3). These items were included in the final questionnaire. The two items measuring the construct Affect were allocated to more than one domain, including Emotion, Social/professional role and identity, and Beliefs about capabilities. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .58; 95% C.I. .44-.70; Table 4), while almost perfect agreement was found for the final 32 items (κ = .90; 95% C.I., .83-.96; Table 4).

Behavioral regulation

The domain Behavioral regulation was defined as 'anything aimed at managing or changing objectively observed or measured actions’[37]. Ten items, including Action planning items also aimed to measure the domain Goals, were included in the DCV to measure Behavioral regulation. None of them were classified to the right domain (Table 3) and therefore Behavioral regulation items were not included in the final questionnaire. The six items measuring the constructs Automaticity and Self-monitoring were allocated to more than one domain including Behavioral regulation, Skills, Goals, and Memory attention, and decision processes. Two judges did not allocate any of the 79 items to the domain. Without these judges taken into account kappa indicated fair agreement (κ = .36; 95% C.I. .20-.52; Table 4).

All items and domains

Overall, moderate agreement was found for the allocation of all 79 items to the 14 domains (κ = .56; 95% C.I. .50-.62; Table 4), while almost perfect agreement was found for the allocation of the final 32 items to the 14 domains (κ = .82; 95% C.I. .79-.85; Table 4).

Discussion

We have developed a TDF-based questionnaire in both English and Dutch able to discriminately assess the majority of domains. For the first time, items have been operationalized to assess TDF domains using theoretical constructs within each domain and these items were judged to be either pure measures of the domain, or else also measuring other domains. Our findings provide an additional level of validation for the content of the TDF: not only do judges agree about the constructs within each domain and the domain structure as demonstrated by Cane et al.[37], but the majority of TDF domains have now been shown to be largely discriminately measurable. These results correspond with Taylor et al.[36, 63] who found good discriminant validity of TDF domains in a questionnaire measuring influences on patient safety behaviors[36] and in the Determinants of Physical Activity Questionnaire[63]. While Taylor et al.[36, 63] used specific items (i.e., related to a specific application), our items are generic and allow for application within a range of different contexts in which implementation research takes place. In summary, the development of our questionnaire provides important evidence of content validity and is a first step towards the development of a valid and reliable questionnaire to measure TDF-based factors underlying healthcare professionals’ specific implementation behaviors.

Of the 79 items assessed, 32 items were able to discriminately measure the following 11 domains: Knowledge, Skills, Social/professional role and identity, Beliefs about capabilities, Optimism, Beliefs about consequences, Intentions, Memory, attention and decision processes, Environmental context and resources, Social influences, and Emotion. For each of these domains at least two items were identified that can be used in the development of a TDF-based questionnaire.

Following judges allocations, items were not able to measure the domains Reinforcement, Goals, and Behavioral regulation. Items intended to measure these domains were allocated to multiple domains or classified to a domain other than the item intended to measure. This may be due to a few reasons. First, it is possible that the items used to operationalize the constructs within these domains were not appropriate, which might be related to the fact that some of Reinforcement and Goals items were newly developed by the researchers rather than previously-validated items. Nevertheless, items intended to measure the domain Behavioral regulation through the constructs Automaticity, Self-monitoring, and Action planning were adapted from previously published questionnaires, and thus it is unlikely that the existing level of validation of items is responsible for challenges in allocating items to particular domains. Second, it might be that items could not be classified to measure these three domains, because the domain definitions were not fit for purpose. This is associated with the finding that five, three, and two judges did not allocate any of the items to, respectively, the domains Reinforcement, Goals, and Behavioral regulation. The findings may also be explained by the use of domain definitions instead of construct definitions to allocate items, while items were previously developed to target individual constructs rather than broader domains. The allocation of items to domain definitions might therefore be influenced by the closeness of the definition of the domain to the definition of its constituent constructs. Finally, it could be that the remaining domains themselves cannot be discriminately measured. This seems a plausible explanation, as the domain Reinforcement is a refinement of the Beliefs about consequences domain and was originally included within the latter domain in the original TDF[31]. It is then perhaps not surprising that the Reinforcement items were judged to be assessing Beliefs about consequences, and arguably, such assignment is theoretically appropriate. Furthermore, the refinement of the domain Motivation and goals of the original TDF[31] into the domains Goals and Intentions in the recent version of the TDF and the classification of multiple goal-related constructs to the domains Goals, Intentions, and Behavioral regulation imply overlap between these domains. Therefore, it is perhaps also not surprising that the items measuring these domains were allocated to all three domains, and thus are not able to discriminately measure them. From a discriminant content validity perspective, taken together these results support keeping to the 12 original domains as a basis for the development of TDF questionnaires. When using the 12-domain framework[31] to develop a TDF-based questionnaire, items measuring the domains Behavioral regulation and Nature of the behaviors should be identified to maintain the comprehensive nature of the TDF. This could be done by selecting domains’ related key constructs as provided by Michie et al.[31] and selecting items from existing validated scales.

Lastly, the findings indicate that further refinement of the final questionnaire is required. In general, the amount of items measuring most of the domains could be increased to at least three items for each domain (at least three items with a loading above .80 will give a reliable component[64]). With regard to the specific domains, the final items measuring the domain Environmental context and resources are framed entirely in terms of the socio-political context, while there may be additional environmental and resources influences that remain unmeasured. The initial version of the questionnaire included items related to characteristics of the innovation, organization, socio-political context, and innovation strategies[16–20], however, only the items assessing the socio-political context were judged to discriminately assess this domain. Lack of discriminant content validity of items measuring characteristics of the innovation, organization, and innovation strategies might be due to our method of developing a generic questionnaire based on factors related to a specific implementation behavior (i.e., the implementation of PA interventions). Moreover, the domain Environmental context and resources is arguably among the least well conceptualized domains of the TDF, which may partly explain challenges that judges faced in allocating items to this domain. Nevertheless, potential users of the final questionnaire may wish to incorporate additional more contextually sensitive items focusing on the environment and resources whilst recognizing that their discriminant content validity has not yet been demonstrated. In the initial questionnaire, items measuring the domain Emotion were adapted from previously published questionnaires. Specifically, items measuring the construct Affect were based on the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule[49] and Stress items were based on the General Health Questionnaire[48]. Items measuring the construct Stress demonstrated to be able to discriminately assess the domain Emotions, while Affect items did not. Therefore, the final questionnaire includes items concerning healthcare professionals’ general feelings (i.e., Stress) instead of their emotions related to performing a specific behavior (i.e., Affect). Yet, when investigating determinants of healthcare professionals’ implementation behaviors, items assessing emotions in relation to performing a specific behavior should also be taken into account as these have been found to be linked to implementation behaviors in previous research[65–67]. Although initial TACT-specific items assessing the construct Affect were not judged to discriminately assess the domain Emotions, potential users of the final questionnaire may want to consider using such items by including other emotions such as pride, empathy[67], fear[65–67], and embarrassment[66]. Furthermore, the assessment of the domain Knowledge could be improved by adding items to test healthcare professionals’ knowledge on a certain implementation behavior[66, 68].

Strengths and limitations of assessing TDF domains using questionnaires

Limitations with regard to the use of the TDF for questionnaire development involve the large amount of domains and underlying constructs that can only be assessed by a large amount of items. Quantitative TDF-based research might preclude measuring all constructs within each domain due to time constraints as described earlier by Amemori et al.[34]. As a result, it is not clear which constructs to choose when measuring a given domain. In this study, constructs were selected based on close relatedness to the content of the domains, being a part of important theories of behavior change, existence of validated scales, and/or relevance to the implementation of PA interventions in routine healthcare as determined in previous studies[43, 44]. However, it is unclear to what extent the constructs that we selected measure the full breadth of the domains instead of a part of them. This questionnaire strove to balance representation of the constructs within the domains with a parsimonious questionnaire that could be feasibly used in the field. However, some domains cover a wider breath of constructs than others and future work could investigate the broader range of constructs within each domain. In addition, the TDF domains are potential behavioral determinants, instead of factors proven to influence implementation behavior and the framework does not specify relationships between domains[30]. On the other hand, quantitative applications of the framework can be beneficial for use in exploratory research and to guide theory selection.

Corresponding with the major rationale for the development of the original TDF, the framework can be used to assess a broad range of factors from a multitude of behavior change theories, helpful when little a priori information is available to base the selection of appropriate theories on. In comparison with other frameworks used in implementation research, e.g.,[16, 17, 20], and empirical work on the introduction of PA interventions in primary healthcare[43, 44] the TDF[37], however, mainly focuses on factors related to the adopting person, instead of taking into account a variety of factors related to characteristics of the innovation, patient, social setting, organizational context, and innovation methods and strategies[16–20]. This implies factors outside psychological behavior change theory are not adequately elaborated in the framework. We believe that these factors may be included in the domain Environmental context and resources or multiple 'environmental’ domains should be incorporated in the TDF.

Strengths and limitations of our methods

While we used a rigorous DCV approach to validate the content of items in the questionnaire, some limitations of our study need to be taken into account. The DCV exercise of allocating 79 items to 14 domains was a challenging task for judges, requiring consideration of multiple possible definitions. This approach is a degree of magnitude more challenging than how DCVs have typically been applied in the past (to a much smaller number of constructs). A larger number of judges and a less complex task would have possibly increased information on discriminant content validity of the items. Major strengths of this study include the sample of academics with expertise on TDF domains and the formulation of items using the 'TACT principle’[38], which allows potential users of the questionnaire to tailor the content to their own target, action, context, and time. However, the operationalization and validation of the domains of the TDF are limited to these specific methods. It could be, for example, that in 'real life’ the validity of the domains would differ from the one perceived by an academic audience. Therefore, this study represents an important first step in the thorough development of a questionnaire to measure TDF-based factors underlying healthcare professionals’ implementation behaviors. As a next step we tested the Determinants of Implementation Behavior Questionnaire (DIBQ) on a sample of 270 healthcare professionals with specification of a particular target, action, context, and time, and showed good construct validity, with the majority of domains showing high internal consistency reliability and discriminant validity (Huijg et al., submitted).

Conclusions

To our knowledge, this study is the first to develop a generic (i.e., formulation of items following the 'TACT principle’[38]) TDF-based questionnaire in both English and Dutch including items which are able to discriminately measure a majority of the domains. The results partly support Cane et al.’s validation of the TDF[37] and suggest that the 12-domain version[31] might be more applicable in developing a TDF-based questionnaire. The items of this questionnaire can be used for the development of a questionnaire to measure TDF-based determinants of healthcare professionals’ specific implementation behaviors. Future research should investigate the concurrent and predictive validity and reliability of such a questionnaire in practice, among a large healthcare professional sample.

In general, a valid TDF-based questionnaire will increase the use of theory in the assessment of barriers and facilitators for implementation problems[31, 69, 70], which can inform the selection of possible techniques that can be used to change healthcare professionals’ behaviors[15, 23, 26]. Consequently, research on the development of a generic TDF questionnaire will improve our understanding of factors influencing healthcare professionals’ implementation and advance theory and methods in implementation research.

Abbreviations

PA:

Physical activity

TDF:

Theoretical domains framework

DCV:

Discriminant content validation

DIBQ:

Determinants of implementation behavior questionnaire

References

  1. Grimshaw JM, Eccles MP, Lavis JN, Hill SJ, Squires JE: Knowledge translation of research findings. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 50-10.1186/1748-5908-7-50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Glasgow RE, Lichtenstein E, Marcus AC: Why don’t we see more translation of health promotion research to practice? Rethinking the efficacy-to-effectiveness transition. Am J Public Health. 2003, 93: 1261-1267. 10.2105/AJPH.93.8.1261.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Haines A, Kuruvilla S, Borchert M: Policy and Practice Bridging the implementation gap between knowledge and action for health. B World Health Organization. 2004, 82: 724-732.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Eakin EG, Glasgow RE, Riley KM: Review of primary care-based physical activity intervention studies. J Fam Practice. 2000, 49: 158-168.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Petrella RJ, Lattanzio CN: Does counseling help patients get practice? Systematic review of the literature. Can Fam Physician. 2002, 48: 72-80.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Sørensen JB, Skovgaard T, Puggaard L: Exercise on prescription in general practice: a systematic review. Scand J Prim Health. 2006, 24: 69-74. 10.1080/02813430600700027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Orrow G, Kinmonth A-L, Sanderson S, Sutton S: Effectiveness of physical activity promotion based in primary care: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2012, 344: e1389-10.1136/bmj.e1389.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Glasgow RE, Eakin EG, Fisher EB, Bacak SJ, Brownson RC: Physician advice and support for physical activity: results from a national survey. Am J Prev Med. 2001, 21: 189-196. 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00350-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Fallon EA, Wilcox S, Laken M: Health care provider advice for African American adults not meeting health behavior recommendations. Prev Chron Dis. 2006, 3: 1-12.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Helmink JHM, Meis JJM, de Weerdt I, Visser FN, de Vries NK, Kremers SPJ: Development and implementation of a lifestyle intervention to promote physical activity and healthy diet in the Dutch general practice setting: the BeweegKuur programme. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2010, 7: 49-10.1186/1479-5868-7-49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Verheijden MW, Bakx JC, Delemarre ICG, Wanders AJ, Woudenbergh NM, Bottema BJAM, van Weel C, van Staveren WA: GPs’ assessment of patients’ readiness to change diet, activity and smoking. Brit J Gen Pract. 2005, 55: 452-457.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Glasgow RE, Bull SS, Gilette C, Klesges LM, Dzewaltowski DA: Behavior change intervention research in healthcare settings: a review of recent reports with emphasis on external validity. Am J Prev Med. 2002, 23: 62-69. 10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00437-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Bartholomew KL, Parcel GS, Kok G, Gottlieb NH, Fernández ME: Planning Health Promotion Programs: An Intervention Mapping Approach. 2011, San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass

    Google Scholar 

  14. Foy R, Eccles M, Grimshaw J: Why does primary care need more implementation research?. Fam Pract. 2001, 18: 353-355. 10.1093/fampra/18.4.353.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Walker A, Johnston M, Pitts N: Changing the behavior of healthcare professionals: the use of theory in promoting the uptake of research findings. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005, 58: 107-112. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Fleuren M, Wiefferink K, Paulussen T: Determinants of innovation within health care organizations: literature review and Delphi study. Int J Qual Health C. 2004, 16: 107-123. 10.1093/intqhc/mzh030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Greenhalgh T, Robert G, Macfarlane F, Bate P, Kyriakidou O: Diffusion of innovations in service organizations: systematic review and recommendations. Milbank Q. 2004, 82: 581-629. 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00325.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Grol R, Wensing M, Eccles M: Improving Patient Care: The Implementation of Change in Clinical Practice. 2005, Oxford: Elsevier

    Google Scholar 

  19. Grol R, Grimshaw J: From best evidence to best practice: effective implementation of change in patients’ care. Lancet. 2003, 362: 1225-1230. 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14546-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Rogers EM: Diffusion of Innovations. 1983, New York: The Free Press

    Google Scholar 

  21. Francis JJ, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ, Grimshaw JM, Johnston M, Hyde C, Stockton C, Brehaut JC, Fergusson D, Eccles MP: Using theories of behaviour to understand transfusion prescribing in three clinical contexts in two countries: development work for an implementation trial. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 70-10.1186/1748-5908-4-70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Grol RPTM, Bosch MC, Hulscher MEJL, Eccles MP, Wensing M: Planning and studying improvement in patient care: the use of theoretical perspectives. Milbank Q. 2007, 85: 93-138. 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2007.00478.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Michie S, Johnston M, Francis J, Hardeman W, Eccles M: From theory to intervention: mapping theoretically derived behavioural determinants to behaviour change techniques. Appl Psychol. 2008, 57: 660-680. 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00341.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Craig P, Dieppe P, Macintyre S, Michie S, Nazareth I, Petticrew M: Developing and evaluationg complex interventions: the new medical research council guidance. BMJ. 2008, 337: a1655-10.1136/bmj.a1655.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Francis J, Johnston M, Bonetti D: Health psychology research advances theory by addressing problems: illustrations from theory-based studies of health professional behaviour. Eur Health Psychol. 2007, 2: 20-25.

    Google Scholar 

  26. French SD, Green SE, O’Connor DA, McKenzie JE, Francis JJ, Michie S, Buchbinder R, Schattner P, Spike N, Grimshaw JM: Developing theory-informed behaviour change interventions to implement evidence into practice: a systematic approach using the Theoretical Domains Framework. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 38-10.1186/1748-5908-7-38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Presseau J, Johnston M, Francis J, Hrisos S, Stamp E, Steen N, Hawthorne G, Grimshaw J, Elovainio M, Hunter M, Eccles M: Theory-based predictors of multiple clinician behaviors in the management of diabetes. J Behav Med. 2013, epub ahead of print

    Google Scholar 

  28. Chaudoir SR, Dugan AG, Barr CHI: Measuring factors affecting implementation of health innovations: a systematic review of structural, organizational, provider, patient, and innovation level. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 22-10.1186/1748-5908-8-22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Rothman AJ: 'Is there nothing more practical than a good theory?’: why innovations and advances in health behavior change will arise if interventions are used to test and refine theory. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2004, 1: 11-10.1186/1479-5868-1-11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Francis JJ, Stockton C, Eccles MP, Johnston M, Cuthbertson BH, Grimshaw JM, Hyde C, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ: Evidence-based selection of theories for designing behaviour change interventions: using methods based on theoretical construct domains to understand clinicians’ blood transfusion behaviour. Brit J Health Psychol. 2009, 14: 625-646. 10.1348/135910708X397025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Michie S, Johnston M, Abraham C, Lawton R, Parker D, Walker A: Making psychological theory useful for implementing evidence based practice: a consensus approach. Qual Saf Health Care. 2005, 14: 26-33. 10.1136/qshc.2004.011155.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Patey AM, Islam R, Francis JJ, Bryson GL, Grimshaw JM: Anesthesiologists’ And surgeons’ perceptions about routine pre-operative testing in low-risk patients: application of the theoretical domains framework (TDF) to identify factors that influence physicians’ decisions to order pre-operative tests. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 52-10.1186/1748-5908-7-52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. McSherry LA, Dombrowski SU, Francis JJ, Murphy J, Martin CM, O’Leary JJ, Sharp L, ATHENS Group: 'It’s a can of worms’: understanding primary care practitioners’ behaviours in relation to HPV using the theoretical domains framework. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 73-10.1186/1748-5908-7-73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Amemori M, Michie S, Korhonen T, Murtomaa H, Kinnunen TH: Assessing implementation difficulties in tobacco use prevention and cessation counselling among dental providers. Implement Sci. 2011, 6: 50-10.1186/1748-5908-6-50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Beenstock J, Sniehotta FF, White M, Bell R, Milne EM, Araujo-Soares V: What helps and hinders midwives in engaging with pregnant women about stopping smoking? A cross-sectional survey of perceived implementation difficulties among midwives in the northeast of England. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 36-10.1186/1748-5908-7-36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Taylor N, Parveen S, Robins V, Slater B, Lawton R: Development and initial validation of the Influences on Patient Safety Behaviours Questionnaire. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 81-10.1186/1748-5908-8-81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S: Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 37-10.1186/1748-5908-7-37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Fishbein M: Attitude and the Prediction of Behavior. Readings in Atttiude Theory and Measurement. Edited by: Fishbein M. 1967, New York: Wiley, 477-492.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Lynn MR: Determination and quantification of content validity. Nurs Res. 1986, 35: 382-385.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Rubio DM, Berg-Weger M, Tebb SS, Lee ES, Rauch S: Objectifying content validity: conducting a content validity study in social work research. Soc Work Res. 2003, 27: 94-104. 10.1093/swr/27.2.94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Ajzen I: The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Dec. 1991, 50: 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Bandura A: Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol Health. 1998, 13: 623-649. 10.1080/08870449808407422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Huijg JM, van der Zouwe N, Gebhardt WA, Crone MR, Verheijden MW, Middelkoop BJC: Introducing physical activity interventions in primary health care: a qualitative study of perceived facilitators and barriers [abstract]. Int J Behav Med. 2012, 19: S1-S341.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Huijg JM, Crone MR, Verheijden MW, van der Zouwe N, Middelkoop BJC, Gebhardt WA: Factors influencing the adoption, implementation, and continuation of physical activity interventions in primary health care: a delphi study. BMC Fam Pract. 2013, 14: 142-10.1186/1471-2296-14-142.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Maes S, Karoly P, de Gucht V, Ruehlman LS, Heiser W: The self regulation skills battery. 2006, Leiden/Phoenix: Leiden University/Arizona State University

    Google Scholar 

  46. Sniehotta FF, Schwarzer R, Scholz U, Schüz B: Action planning and coping planning for long-term lifestyle change: theory and assessment. Eur J Soc Psycho. 2005, 35: 565-576. 10.1002/ejsp.258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Gardner B, Abraham C, Lally P, de Bruijn G-J: Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index. Int J Behav Nutr Phys. 2012, 9: 102-10.1186/1479-5868-9-102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Goldberg DP, Blackwell B: Psychiatric illness in general practice: a detailed study using a new method of case identification. Brit Med J. 1970, 1: 439-443.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Thompson ER: Development and Validation of an Internationally Reliable Short-Form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). J Cross Cult Psychol. 2007, 38: 227-242. 10.1177/0022022106297301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Frese M: Social support as a moderator of the relationship between work stressors and psychological dysfunctioning: a longitudinal study with objective measures. J Occup Health Psych. 1999, 4: 179-192.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Derryberry D, Reed MA: Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111: 225-236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW: Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994, 67: 1063-1078.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Cialdini R, Kallgren C, Reno R: A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: A Theoretical Refinement and Reevaluation of the Role of Norms in Human Behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1991, San Diego: Academic Press, 202-232.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Dixon D, Pollard B, Johnston M: What does the chronic pain grade questionnaire measure?. Pain. 2007, 130: 249-253. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Dixon D, Johnston M, McQueen M, Court-Brown C: The disabilities of the Arm, shoulder and hand questionnaire (DASH) can measure the impairment, activity limitations and participation restriction constructs from the international classification of functioning, disability and health (ICF). BMC Musculoskel Dis. 2008, 9: 114-10.1186/1471-2474-9-114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. American Psychological Association (APA): APA Dictionary of Psychology. 2007, Washington: American Psychological Association

    Google Scholar 

  57. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y: Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Roy Stat Soc B Met. 1995, 57: 289-300.

    Google Scholar 

  58. IBM Corp: IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 19.0. 2010, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp

    Google Scholar 

  59. Conger A: Integration and generalization of kappas for multiple raters. Psychol Bull. 1980, 88: 322-328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Landis JR, Koch GG: The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977, 33: 159-174. 10.2307/2529310.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. R Development Core Team R: R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2012, Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing

    Google Scholar 

  62. Falissard B: Psy: Various Procedures Used in Psychometry. 2012, : R Package Version 1.1

    Google Scholar 

  63. Taylor N, Lawton R, Connor M: Development and initial validation of the determinants of physical activity questionnaire. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2013, 10: 74-10.1186/1479-5868-10-74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Stevens J: Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences. 1992, Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  65. Taylor N, Lawton R, Slater B, Foy R: The demonstration of a theory-based approach to the design of localized patient safety interventions. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 123-10.1186/1748-5908-8-123.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Dyson J, Lawton R, Jackson C, Cheater F: Development of a theory-based instrument to identify barriers and levers to best hand hygiene practice among healthcare practitioners. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 111-10.1186/1748-5908-8-111.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Dyson J, Lawton R, Jackson C, Cheater F: Does the use of a theoretical approach tell us more about hand hygiene behavior? The barriers and levers to hand hygiene. J Infect Prev. 2011, 12: 17-24. 10.1177/1757177410384300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Bonetti D, Johnston M, Clarkson JE, Grimshaw J, Pitts NB, Eccles M, Steen N, Thomas R, Maclennan G, Glidewell L, Walker A: Applying psychologial theories to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of placing preventive fissure sealants. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 25-10.1186/1748-5908-5-25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Davies P, Walker AE, Grimshaw JM: A systematic review of the use of theory in the design of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies and interpretation of the results of rigorous evaluations. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 14-10.1186/1748-5908-5-14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Grimshaw JM, Thomas RE, MacLennan G, Fraser C, Ramsay CR, Vale L, Whitty P, Eccles MP, Matowe L, Shirran L, Wensing M, Dijkstra R, Donaldson C: Effectiveness and efficiency of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies. Health Technol Assess. 2004, 8: 1-72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Furthermore, the authors wish to acknowledge Emma Massey for her involvement in the back-translation of the questionnaire. This research was funded by the European Health Psychology Society Tandem Grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johanna M Huijg.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JP and JMH are equally responsible for the design of the study, development of the questionnaire, and data analysis and interpretation. JMH collected the data and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. Subsequent drafts of the manuscript were written together. WAG and MRC were involved in the development of the questionnaire and critically revised the manuscript. ED was involved in data analysis and interpretation, and commented on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Huijg, J.M., Gebhardt, W.A., Crone, M.R. et al. Discriminant content validity of a theoretical domains framework questionnaire for use in implementation research. Implementation Sci 9, 11 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-9-11

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-9-11

Keywords